This site is intended for healthcare professionals only

Communicating with people who have limited English

Trish Greenhalgh

Around 340 different languages are spoken in London alone, and a high (but not formally quantified) proportion of healthcare consultations take place across a language barrier (Baker and Eversley, 2000). Other cities in the UK have comparable challenges, as evidenced by a large ‘grey literature’ on interpreting need by acute and primary care trusts. People from ethnic minorities have a higher prevalence of diabetes than the indigenous white British population, and diabetes also disproportionately affects the poor, so people with diabetes who are from ethnic minorities are less likely to be educated and fluent in English (Greenhalgh et al, 2001).

Increasingly, language need in any locality is diverse (many different languages are spoken) and dynamic (the languages change every few months) – often to the extent that provision fails to keep up with the demographics of the population (Baker and Eversley, 2000; Salt, 2005; Eversley, 2005). Official estimates of the burden of need for interpreting services in many areas is high and rising, especially in deprived inner-city areas (Baker and Eversley, 2000; Atkinson et al, 2001; Gerrish, 2001; DoH, 2002; McPake et al, 2002), and that access to such services is generally inadequate, especially for the most vulnerable and hard-to-reach groups (Jones and Gill, 1998; Burnett and Fassil, 2000; Atkinson et al, 2001; Gerrish, 2001; Hertog, 2001; DoH, 2002; McPake et al, 2002; Murphy, 2004).

The NHS’s commitment – and the evidence base for it
The NHS has expressed its commitment to providing an equitable service defined by need, including providing a professional interpreter to any individual if needed (DoH, 2004). However, ‘translation’ (with a focus on the words spoken) has tended to take precedence in policy circles over ‘advocacy’ (which acknowledges that a limited English speaker may also have support and access needs, and that the health professional may lack key areas of cultural knowledge or competence). Expenditure on NHS interpreting services is rising rapidly, and questions have been asked about whether or not this is a good use of resources (Adams, 2007; Jones, 2007). What is the evidence for and against the provision of such services?

Research has consistently shown that when a professional interpreter is provided for a consultation, quality of care and satisfaction with care are greater than in comparable situations when no interpreter, or an ad hoc interpreter, is provided (Karliner et al, 2007; Flores, 2005a; Green et al, 2005a). The use of ad hoc interpreters (for example, a receptionist or fellow patient) or family member interpreters is common in many healthcare situations with limited English speakers. Accuracy of translation may be low, even with professionally qualified interpreters, but even more so with lay interpreters (Cambridge, 1999; Elderkin-Thompson et al, 2001; DoH, 2002; McPake et al, 2002).

However, some research has challenged the assumption that a professional interpreter is always the best option. While family member interpreting is clearly inappropriate in certain situations (a child interpreting for a psychosexual consultation for example, or the husband for a young wife’s unexplained bruises), it may be highly valued and seen as effective in less complex or emotionally-charged situations – partly because family members may be easier for the individual to get hold of, and partly because they may shift the power balance in the patient’s favour (Angelelli, 2005; Green et al, 2005a; Green et al, 2005b; Greenhalgh et al, 2006; Robb and Greenhalgh, 2006). One qualitative study concluded that young people who interpret for family and friends ‘wished their work to be conceptualised not as merely “inappropriate and inadequate interpreting”, but as a varied contribution to the informal economy of health care that ranged from simple translation to complex mediation between families, the wider community and the healthcare system’ (Green et al, 2005b).

New technologies offer opportunities beyond conventional face-to-face interpreting, such as phone or video interpreting. Evaluation of these different models has produced mixed findings, with no model clearly best for all people in all circumstances (DoH, 2003; Jones et al, 2003; Silvera and Kapasi, 2004; Garcia et al, 2004).

The risks of poor communication with limited-English speakers
Failure to provide an effective interpreting service is increasingly seen in terms of the risk and safety agenda. For example, this may account for failure of vulnerable groups to access the services they need (why seek help when you will not understand or be understood?) as well as for a rising proportion of medical errors, such as: administering medicines to which the patient is allergic; giving inappropriate vaccinations to children; failure to offer cancer screening to eligible and at-risk individuals; dismissing early symptoms of cancer as ‘non-specific complaints’; people not being aware of what their illness is or how to take their medicines; and procedures and operations being undertaken without informed consent (Chief Medical Officer, 2001; Angelelli, 2005; Flores et al, 2005b; Green et al, 2005a; Abbe et al, 2006; De Alba and Sweningson, 2006; Greenhalgh et al, 2006; Bradshaw et al, 2007; Hunt and de Voogd, 2007; Phokeo and Hyman, 2007).

Economic analyses suggest that although interpreting services is costly, they may nevertheless prove to be cost effective owing to the high impact on quality of care and reduction in error (Jacobs et al, 2004). Medic–olegal advice in the USA is often ‘pay now [for effective interpreting services] or pay later [in a lawsuit]’ (Ku and Flores, 2005). There were no available studies specifically relating to risk in diabetes, though it is not difficult to imagine how poor communication in this setting might lead to loss of limb, loss of sight, or major foetal abnormality.

More subtle than the direct risk to life and limb, but perhaps as important in the longer term, are issues of trust and power. Language and cultural barriers, even when an interpreter is present (but especially if one is not), distort communication and may undermine clinician–patient trust (Angelelli, 2005; Green et al, 2005a; Greenhalgh et al, 2006; Robb and Greenhalgh, 2006; Meeuwesen et al, 2006; Schouten and Meeuwesen, 2006). Language barriers also shift the power balance in the clinical consultation away from the patient. Typically, clinicians control the agenda in the clinical consultation since they control the allocation of time and the issuing of a prescription or referral. A vulnerable and inarticulate patient lacks the power to negotiate – a critical role of the interpreter or advocate, in addition to translating, is to get the patient’s agenda on the table (Greenhalgh et al, 2006; Robb and Greenhalgh, 2006).

Communication is not just about language
It is important not to see communication with limited-English speakers purely in terms of linguistic barriers. Health-seeking behaviour (or the absence of it) is shaped by a person’s cultural background, belief systems and identity, and by their past experiences in their country of origin. A particularly important issue here may be past experience of poorly developed and low-quality primary care services, which shape the expectation that all illness should be referred to a ‘proper’ (secondary care) doctor (Greenhalgh et al, 2006; Robb and Greenhalgh, 2006). Lack of ‘system knowledge’ often underlies poor access to services, especially among recent immigrants, refugees and asylum seekers, since such individuals often don’t know where to start looking for help when they are ill, or how to get into the system (Greenhalgh et al, 2006; O’Donnell et al, 2007).

Limited-English speakers are more likely to be poor, inadequately housed, lacking access to basic health services, and to have multiple co-morbidity (LaVeist, 2005; Sue and Dhindsa, 2006). A history of severe mental trauma is common in refugees and asylum seekers. In one recent study, for example, an interpreter from a war-torn African country estimated that over 90% of her female patients had been raped (Robb and Greenhalgh, 2006).

Limited-English speakers are also more likely to lack functional health literacy, defined by the WHO as ‘the cognitive and social skills that determine the motivation and ability of individuals to gain access to, understand, and use, information in ways that promote and maintain good health’. The concept of health literacy embraces a number of key attributes including reading and numeracy in relation to health information, the capacity to use health information, and the ability to ‘perform in the patient role’. These, in turn, rest on wider literacy and numeracy skills as well as health-related experience – the latter of which achieves two things: exposure to the medical vernacular, and building the relevant cognitive schemas within which new health information makes sense (Speros, 2005; Coulter and Ellins, 2006).

Health literacy correlates closely with healthy lifestyle choices, concordance with medication, overall cost of health care, length of stay in hospital, and outcome in a wide range of conditions (Andrus and Roth, 2002; Wilson, 2003; Speros, 2005; Coulter and Ellins, 2006; Sentell and Halpin, 2006). Many (and in some studies, all) ethnic differences in health outcome are explained by differences in health literacy (Schillinger et al, 2004; Borrell et al, 2006; Sarkar et al, 2006; Sentell and Halpin, 2006).

Organising services and developing staff
Providing an interpreter for every consultation with a limited-English speaker is a highly complex exercise in the organisation and delivery of services, and some health care organisations are more efficient and effective in this than others (Brach et al, 2005; Greenhalgh et al, 2007). Despite many studies showing that interpreters are often under-trained for their role, there is remarkably little research on the training needs of interpreters or bilingual health advocates (Angelelli, 2005). One study has shown that interpreters can be trained in disease knowledge (for example, through half-day release courses and studying videotaped consultations), and such training improves the accuracy of translation (McCabe et al, 2006).

The job of many interpreters involves an inherent role conflict. The role of ‘impartial translator’ requires different competencies (and, importantly, a different identity) than the role of ‘bilingual health advocate’. The former is a contemporary role, linked to the formal division of labour in a professional bureaucracy and based on what has been called the ‘conduit model’ (Dysart-Gale, 2005) or ‘bilingual parrot’ (Greenhalgh et al, 2006). The latter is a more traditional role, linked to ‘kinship’ ties of shared cultural background, ethnicity and sometimes gender (a role that embraces both ‘witness’, ‘ally’ and ‘cultural broker’). In a study undertaken by the author’s own team, when describing positive interpreted consultations, patients often used expressions such as ‘like my daughter’ or ‘like a sister’, suggesting that the intersubjective bond based on ‘kinship’ ties is important (Angelelli, 2005; Dysart-Gale, 2005; Greenhalgh et al, 2006). Professional interpreters often experience considerable role conflict and feel pulled between the professional territory of the doctor and the ‘lifeworld’ of the patient (Angelelli, 2005; Dysart-Gale, 2005; Greenhalgh et al, 2006). In some situations (for example, end-of-life care), the appropriate balance between ‘impartial translator’ and ‘cultural broker’ shifts towards the latter (Norris et al, 2005).

The use of interpreters depends on the willingness of health professionals to work with them. The author’s team has shown that clinicians vary significantly in their willingness to use professional interpreters and in their competence in communicating through (and with) an interpreter; approximately half of all GP surgeries in London, for example, do not use the interpreting service at all (Gerrish et al, 2004; Karliner et al, 2004; Greenhalgh et al, 2007).

Interpreters and bilingual health advocates have a potentially important role to play in educating health professionals and increasing their cultural awareness, helping patients ‘navigate’ the healthcare system, and providing education and support for people with long-term illness through new models of care (Greenhalgh and Collard, 2003; Greenhalgh et al, 2005a; Greenhalgh et al, 2005b; Wu et al, 2006). A randomised trial of bilingual health advocate-led group education for diabetes through oral story telling in Newham, London, has recently been completed; this study will report shortly.

Implications for practice
There is no simple formula for effective communication with limited-English speakers, but the following recommendations are supported by evidence.

  • All GP practices and PCTs should document language need, map this to burden of illness and produce aggregated, regularly updated data on this.
  • Cost-constrained interpreting and advocacy services (which will probably always be unable to fully meet demand) should be targeted towards those whose need is greatest, including those with multiple comorbidity or complex social needs.
  • The wide variability between GP practices in their willingness to use the available interpreting service is unacceptable. It is surely unacceptable that some GP surgeries are still allowed to say ‘we don’t have interpreters here’.
  • The professional development and training of interpreters and bilingual health advocates requires attention at national policy level, but there is also much that primary care teams and PCTs can do on a relatively small scale to develop these important staff.
  • New service models in which interpreters, advocates, health navigators and other staff are used in new roles, and especially in ways that meet local needs and priorities, should be explored. It is surely a priority to consider how interpreters might be used as educators to improve the cultural competence of medical, nursing and pharmacy staff, and how the NHS might link with the voluntary, NGO (non-governmental organization) and private sectors to provide effective interpreting and advocacy services in today’s mixed economy of healthcare.

Conclusion
In conclusion, contemporary diabetes care in many parts of the UK requires attention to language and communication barriers. These barriers include – but are not limited to – the task of translating what is said. The astute primary care practitioner will also question what cultural expectations and fears the patient brings, and the nature of the patient’s lifeworld. Providing interpreting services is a huge management challenge, and the professional development of the interpreting and advocacy workforce is an important aspect of this.   

REFERENCES:

Abbe M, Simon C, Angiolillo A et al (2006) Pediatric Blood & Cancer 47: 819–24
Adams K (2007) BMJ 334: 398
Andrus MR, Roth MT (2002) Pharmacotherapy 22: 282–302
Angelelli C (2005) Medical Interpreting and Cross-cultural Communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Atkinson M et al (2001) Systematic Review of Ethnicity and Health Service Access for London. Centre for Health Services Studies, with Mary Seacole Research Centre and CRER, Coventry
Baker P, Eversley J (2000) Multilingual Capital. The Languages of London’s School children and their Relevance to Economic, Social and Educational Policies. Battlebridge Publications, London
Borrell LN, Dallo FJ, White K (2006) American Journal of Public Health 96: 1637–42
Brach C, Fraser I, Paez K (2005) Health Affairs (Millwood) 24: 424–34
Bradshaw M, Tomany-Korman S, Flores G (2007) Pediatrics 120: e225–35
Burnett A, Fassil Y (2000) Meeting the Health Needs of Refugees and Asylum Seekers in the UK. An Information and Resource Pack for Health Workers. DoH, London
Cambridge J (1999) The Translator 5: 201–19
Chief Medical Officer (2001) An Organisation with a Memory. DoH, London
Coulter A, Ellins J (2006) Improving health literacy. Patient Focussed Interventions – A review of the evidence. Picker Institute, Oxford
De Alba I, Sweningson JM (2006) Cancer Detection and Prevention 30: 292–6
DoH (2002) Ask if it Hurts: A Qualitative Study of Interpretation Services in the National Health Service. DoH, London
DoH (2003) Review Of Service Delivery Models for Translation And Interpretation. DoH, London
DoH (2004) Guidance on developing local communication support services and strategies. DoH, London
Dysart-Gale D (2005) Health Communication 17: 91–103
Elderkin-Thompson V, Silver RC, Waitzkin H (2001) Social Science & Medicine 52: 1343–58
Eversley J (2005) Tower Hamlets language study: speakers of different languages in Tower Hamlets. PPRE, London
Flores G (2005a) Medical Care Research & Review 62: 255–99
Flores G, Abreu M, Tomany-Korman SC (2005b) Public Health Report 120: 418–30
Garcia EA, Roy LC, Okada PJ et al (2004) Pediatric Emergency Care 20: 373–8
Gerrish K (2001) Journal of Advanced Nursing 33: 566–74
Gerrish K, Chau R, Sobowale A, Birks E (2004) Health & Social Care in the Community 12: 407–13
Green AR, Ngo-Metzger Q, Legedza AT et al (2005a) Journal of General and Internal Medicine 20: 1050–6
Green J, Free C, Bhavnani V, Newman T (2005b) Social Science and Medicine 60: 2097–110
Greenhalgh T, Griffiths T, Thomas K et al (2001) Reducing inequalities in diabetes. Report of the Expert Reference Group for the National Service Framework for Diabetes. Department of Health, London
Greenhalgh T, Collard A (2003) Sharing stories – a new approach to multiprofessional training in diabetes care. BMJ Publishing Group, London
Greenhalgh T, Collard A, Begum N (2005a) Practical Diabetes International 22: 125–9
Greenhalgh T, Collard A, Begum N (2005b) BMJ 330: 628
Greenhalgh T, Robb N, Scambler G (2006) Social Science & Medicine 63: 1170–87
Greenhalgh T, Voisey C, Robb N (2007) Sociology of Health and Illness 29: 931–54
Hertog E (2001) Aequitas: Access to Justice across Language and Culture in the EU. Lessius Hogeschool, Antwerp
Hunt LM, de Voogd KB (2007) Journal of General Internal Medicine 22: 598–605
Jacobs EA, Shepard DS, Suaya JA, Stone EL (2004) American Journal of Public Health 94: 866–9
Jones D, Gill P (1998) BMJ 316: 1476
Jones D, Gill P, Harrison R (2003) Journal of Telemedicine and Telecare 9: 51–6
Jones D (2007) BMJ 334: 399
Karliner LS, Perez-Stable EJ, Gildengorin G (2004) Journal of General Internal Medicine 19: 175–83
Karliner LS, Jacobs EA, Chen AH, Mutha S (2007) Health Services Research 42: 727–54
Ku L, Flores G (2005) Health Affairs (Millwood) 24: 435–44
LaVeist TA (2005) Journal of Urban Health 82: iii26–34
McCabe M, Gohdes D, Morgan F et al (2006) Diabetes Education 32: 714–20
McPake J et al (2002) Translating, Interpreting and Communication Support Services Across the Public Sector in Scotland. A Literature Review. Scottish Executive Central Research Unit, Edinburgh
Meeuwesen L, Harmsen JA, Bernsen RM, Bruijnzeels MA (2006) Social Science and Medicine 63: 2407–17
Murphy S (2004) Journal of Medical Practice and Management 20: 23–7
Norris WM, Wenrich MD, Nielsen EL (2005) Journal of Palliative Medicine 8: 1016–24
O’Donnell CA, Higgins M, Chauhan R, Mullen K (2007) BMC Health Serv Res 7: 75
Phokeo V, Hyman I (2007) American Journal of Health-System Pharmacy 64: 423–9
Robb N, Greenhalgh T (2006) Journal of Health Organization and Management 20: 434–55
Salt J (2005) Current trends in international migration in Europe. Council of Europe, Strasbourg
Sarkar U, Fisher L, Schillinger D (2006) Diabetes Care 29: 823–9
Schouten BC, Meeuwesen L (2006) Cultural differences in medical communication: A review of the literature. Patient Education and Counselling 64: 21–36
Schillinger D, Bindman A, Wang F (2004) Patient Education & Counseling 52: 315–23
Sentell TL, Halpin HA (2006) Journal of General Internal Medicine 21: 862–6
Silvera M, Kapasi R (2004) Review Of Service Delivery Models For Translation And Interpretation. 2nd ed. DoH, London
Speros C (2005) Health literacy: concept analysis. Journal of Advanced Nursing 50: 633–40
Sue S, Dhindsa MK (2006) Health Education & Behaviour 33: 459–69
Wilson JF (2003) Annals of Internal Medicine 139: 875–8
Wu AC, Leventhal JM, Ortiz J (2006) Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine 160: 1145–50

Related content
;
Free for all UK & Ireland healthcare professionals

Sign up to all DiabetesontheNet journals

 

By clicking ‘Subscribe’, you are agreeing that DiabetesontheNet.com are able to email you periodic newsletters. You may unsubscribe from these at any time. Your info is safe with us and we will never sell or trade your details. For information please review our Privacy Policy.

Are you a healthcare professional? This website is for healthcare professionals only. To continue, please confirm that you are a healthcare professional below.

We use cookies responsibly to ensure that we give you the best experience on our website. If you continue without changing your browser settings, we’ll assume that you are happy to receive all cookies on this website. Read about how we use cookies.